The  latter  is  particularly  important  as  a  number  of  
studies  ( including  that  of  Oborne  &  Holton  ) appear  to  overestimate  
the  sensitivity  of  global  measures  such  as  reading  comprehension  or  
measures  of  speed  based  on  sentences  or  even  whole  texts .sx   On  
balance  , the  evidence  points  to  a  genuine  disadvantage  in  
processing  text  on  a  VDU  , particularly  when  task  demands  are  high  
( Wright  &  Lickorish  , 1983  ; Zwahlen  , Hartman  , Ranjarajula  , &  
Escontrela  , 1985) .sx   However  , there  is  no  generally  accepted  
explanation  of  the  effects .sx   Visual  discomfort  and  headache  appear  
to  be  a  consequence  of  staring  at  a  flickering  striped  surface  
( Wilkins  , 1985a) .sx   Changes  in  reading  speed  must  obviously  relate  in  
some  way  to  the  distribution  of  eye  movements  over  the  screen  or  
page  , involving  , for  example  , changes  in  the  frequency  and/or  
duration  of  fixations  and  the  extent  of  saccades .sx   However  , these  
variables  have  not  previously  been  systematically  examined  using  a  
task  approaching  normal  reading .sx   
  An  important  observation  regarding  the  effects  of  flicker  on  
eye  movement  control  was  made  by  Wilkins  ( 1985b) .sx   He  examined  eye  
movements  as  subjects  made  repeated  saccades  between  two  letters  
embedded  in  a  piece  of  text .sx   The  display  was  illuminated  at  50  Hz  
or  100  Hz .sx   The  results  showed  reliably  longer  saccades  at  50  Hz  
than  at  100  Hz  , the  size  of  the  effect  being  about  one  character  
position .sx   A  further  , similar  experiment  contrasted  100-Hz  with  
20-KHz  flicker  , and  again  saccades  were  longer  at  the  lower  rate  -  
although  the  effect  was  only  reliable  when  the  target  letters  were  
wide  apart .sx   Wilkins  is  tempted  to  interpret  this  finding  as  
indicating  an  increase  in  saccade  length  in  flicker  , but  it  is  
important  to  note  that  absolute  accuracy  differed  in  the  two  
conditions .sx   At  50  Hz  saccades  overshot  the  target  slightly  , and  at  
100  Hz  slightly  undershot  it .sx   Without  a  baseline  condition  of  
steady  illumination  it  is  not  possible  to  interpret  the  results  in  
terms  of  a  flicker  -  induced  extension  of  saccade  length .sx   
Further  , since  Wilkins  employed  an  external  event  ( a  tone  ) as  a  
signal  for  a  saccade  to  take  place  , the  possibility  arises  that  the  
obtained  inaccuracy  could  relate  to  an  increased  proportion  of  
prematurely  triggered  saccades  ( this  is  discussed  in  greater  detail  
below) .sx   
  There  are  , in  fact  , two  sources  of  evidence  converging  on  the  
prediction  that  interfering  transient  events  , of  the  kind  that  may  
arise  in  flicker  , may  disturb  eye  movement  control .sx   First  , a  study  
by  Becker  , Kieffer  , and  Jurgens  ( 1987  ) showed  that  both  auditory  
and  visual  signals  delivered  after  the  onset  of  a  saccade  may  
modify  its  extent  and  velocity .sx   The  effect  of  the  transient  signal  
varied  somewhat  between  subjects  but  led  overall  to  a  change  in  the  
final  target  location  , shortening  the  saccade's  length .sx   Similarly  , 
Neary  and  Wilkins  ( 1989  ) showed  that  when  saccades  are  made  between  
two  points  on  a  raster  display  with  a  short-persistence  
  phosphor  , transient  'ghost  images'  may  be  
seen  in  the  vicinity  of  the  target  point .sx   When  lines  rather  than  
points  are  used  , the  fixated  target  may  appear  to  tilt  momentarily  
as  a  result  of  the  top-to-bottom  scan  of  the  raster  display .sx   
Clearly  , these  data  also  show  that  brief  stimulation  during  a  
saccade  can  interfere  with  the  normal  suppression  processes .sx   The  
second  line  of  evidence  derives  from  the  possibility  that  saccades  
may  be  triggered  by  transient  external  events  that  are  
independent  of  the  process  of  saccade  computation  itself .sx   We  assume  
that  when  such  external  triggers  occur  , saccades  are  launched  
towards  their  destination  on  the  basis  of  the  current  ( incomplete  ) 
state  of  the  on-going  computation  ( O'Regan  , 1989) .sx   For  each  
saccade  , target  location  is  continually  updated  on  the  basis  of  
available  visual  information .sx   This  becomes  more  refined  with  time  
and  is  modulated  by  attentional  processes  ( Deubel  , Wolf  , &  Hauske  , 
1984) .sx   Thus  , high  spatial  frequency  information  becomes  available  
later  than  low-frequency  information .sx   A  'premature'  trigger  will  
result  in  a  saccade  launched  effectively  to  the  available  centre  of  
gravity  of  the  target  region  as  a  whole  ( Findlay  , 1981  , 1982) .sx   In  
principle  , such  an  effect  may  result  in  either  undershoot  or  
overshoot  , depending  on  the  particular  visual  configuration  ( see  
O'Regan  , 1989  , for  an  interesting  discussion  of  this  point  ) , but  it  
is  important  to  note  that  in  normal  reading  , for  the  majority  of  
cases  , large  saccades  over  the  page  or  screen  surface  are  
likely  to  be  directed  towards  a  boundary  , in  which  case  the  'centre  
of  gravity'  effect  will  lead  to  a  preponderance  of  undershoots .sx   
  It  is  possible  that  both  mechanisms  operate  while  a  person  is  
reading  flickering  text .sx   By  definition  , saccades  interrupted  in  
flight  will  be  of  reduced  extent .sx   Similarly  , but  for  a  quite  
different  reason  , large  saccades  prematurely  triggered  will  tend  to  
land  short .sx   On  the  other  hand  , if  the  effects  of  premature  
triggering  on  saccades  of  normal  extent  is  to  increase  the  
frequency  of  both  under-  and  over-shoot  , the  overall  result  will  be  
a  change  in  the  variability  in  landing  position .sx   To  this  will  
be  added  the  possible  effect  of  interference  during  a  saccade  , 
although  this  may  be  more  likely  to  affect  those  saccades  directed  
to  remote  targets  , which  will  be  longer  in  duration .sx   The  key  
question  is  whether  an  increase  in  the  number  of  mislocated  
saccades  produced  in  this  way  has  important  consequences  for  the  
reader ?sx   The  results  from  a  number  of  recent  eye  movement  studies  
suggest  a  positive  answer .sx   
  It  is  now  well  established  that  the  eye  movement  control  system  
is  relatively  intolerant  of  deviations  from  what  is  variously  
termed  the  'convenient'  or  'preferred'  viewing  position  for  a  given  
word  ( O'Regan  &  Levy-Schoen  , 1987  ; Rayner  , 1979) .sx   Under  laboratory  
conditions  , involving  the  inspection  of  words  in  isolation  , the  
'convenient  viewing  position'  for  the  majority  of  words  of  average  
length  lies  slightly  to  the  left  of  the  centre .sx   The  penalty  for  
landing  outside  this  point  is  approximately  20  msec  per  letter .sx   
However  , this  figure  is  a  statistical  average  , primarily  resulting  
from  increases  in  the  probability  of  making  additional  corrective  
saccades  when  the  eye  falls  outside  the  convenient  viewing  
position .sx   The  degree  to  which  these  effects  are  found  in  normal  
reading  has  recently  been  examined  by  McConkie  , Kerr  , Reddix  and  
Zola  ( 1988) .sx   The  'preferred  viewing  position'  ( i.e.  where  the  eye  
actually  falls  , as  distinct  from  what  may  be  the  best  place  for  it  
to  be  ) shows  a  similar  curvilinear  relationship  with  letter  
position  , although  the  curve  is  much  flatter  ( see  also  Vitu  &  
O'Regan  , 1989) .sx   When  account  is  taken  of  the  'launch  site'  ( the  
distance  from  the  target  word  ) from  which  a  saccade  is  made  , the  
probability  of  landing  on  a  particular  letter  , for  words  of  all  
lengths  , is  a  roughly  normal  function  about  the  word  centre .sx   
McConkie  et  al.  ( 1988  ) identify  a  number  of  visual  and  oculomotor  
processes  that  might  underlie  the  obtained  variability  in  landing  
position  , but  for  present  purposes  it  is  only  necessary  to  note  ( a  ) 
that  a  'preferred  viewing  position'  ( i.e.  the  word  centre  ) exists  
for  normal  reading  ; and  ( b  ) that  an  increase  in  variability  about  
this  position  or  a  systematic  shift  away  from  it  will  change  the  
probability  of  additional  corrective  saccades  ( McConkie  , Kerr  , 
Reddix  , Zola  , &  Jacobs  , 1989) .sx   
  In  previous  work  we  have  shown  that  one  class  of  eye  movements  
distinguishes  a  successful  reading  style .sx   This  involves  the  use  of  
large  saccades  ( e.g.  spanning  more  than  40  character  positions  ) to  
reinspect  selected  parts  of  previously  read  text .sx   The  reader  can  , 
by  this  means  , locate  antecedents  of  anaphoric  expressions  ( Murray  
&  Kennedy  , 1988  ) ; deal  with  local  syntactic  ambiguity  ( Kennedy  &  
Murray  , 1984  ; Kennedy  , Murray  , Jennings  , &  Reid  , 1989  ) ; or  
reanalyse  an  initially  incorrect  semantic  reading  ( Pynte  , Kennedy  , 
Murray  , &  Courrieu  , 1988) .sx   Since  the  targets  of  such  reinspections  
are  remote  , their  control  cannot  be  based  on  physical  
identification  of  the  target :sx   it  must  , rather  , depend  on  the  reader  
maintaining  a  form  of  spatially  coded  information  ( Kennedy  , 
1987) .sx   Obviously  , if  flicker  acts  to  perturb  saccade  control  , this  
class  of  saccades  provides  an  ideal  litmus  test  of  its  effect  since  
the  saccades  in  question  are  large  , increasing  the  probability  of  
disturbance  ; they  have  a  distinct  role  to  play  in  the  reader's  
process  of  comprehension  ; and  their  use  effectively  characterizes  
an  efficient  reading  style .sx   
  The  experiment  reported  here  examined  the  effects  of  flicker  on  
landing  position  and  on  a  number  of  other  eye  movement  measures  
under  experimental  conditions  that  called  for  the  execution  of  a  
number  of  large  reinspecting  saccades .sx   Following  Wilkins  , two  
flicker  frequencies  were  used  ( 50  Hz  and  100  Hz  ) , but  in  the  
present  study  a  control  condition  was  introduced  using  steady  
illumination .sx   Two  populations  were  studied  -  a  group  of  typists  who  
reported  relatively  severe  symptoms  of  headache  and  visual  fatigue  
associated  with  using  word-processors  ; and  a  group  of  students  with  
little  experience  of  VDUs .sx   The  procedure  employed  was  based  on  that  
of  Kennedy  and  Murray  ( 1987) .sx   Subjects  read  a  sentence  displayed  on  
a  single  horizontal  line  , and  this  was  then  followed  by  the  
presentation  of  a  stimulus  word  to  the  right  of  the  sentence  on  the  
same  line .sx   The  subject's  task  was  to  indicate  whether  or  not  the  
word  was  present  in  the  displayed  sentence .sx   This  task  produces  a  
relatively  high  number  of  large  saccades  , involving  both  
reinspections  of  the  target  word  and  returns  to  the  stimulus .sx   It  is  
a  situation  more  closely  approximating  normal  reading  than  that  
used  by  Wilkins  ( 1985b  ) in  that  it  encouraged  , but  did  not  demand  , 
that  specific  reinspections  took  place .sx   The  task  is  thus  a  good  
analogue  of  some  aspects  of  normal  reading  since  when  subjects  do  
reinspect  , it  is  to  a  variable  target  position  that  is  not  
pre-defined  ( i.e.  not  involving  a  visual  search  ) and  yet  is  
specifiable  for  each  item .sx   
  Four  hypotheses  were  tested :sx   ( 1  ) that  flicker  will  act  to  alter  
the  distribution  of  landing  positions  within  the  word  in  the  'first  
pass'  ( i.e.  in  the  phase  of  the  experimental  procedure  before  
presentation  of  the  stimulus  word  ) ; ( 2  ) that  flicker  will  act  to  
alter  the  accuracy  of  large  saccades  directed  towards  a  remote  
target  , causing  an  increased  proportion  to  fall  short  ; ( 3  ) that  the  
effects  of  an  increased  probability  of  mislocated  saccades  will  be  
an  increase  in  the  number  of  small  'corrective'  eye  movements  ; ( 4  ) 
that  the  disruptive  effects  of  flicker  will  be  greater  in  a  sample  
of  typists  reporting  symptoms  of  visual  discomfort .sx   The  first  three  
hypotheses  arise  naturally  from  the  earlier  discussion .sx   The  fourth  
requires  some  additional  justification .sx   Whether  or  not  an  increase  
in  the  proportion  of  mislocated  saccades  seriously  disrupts  the  
reader  may  relate  to  both  subject  and  task  demands .sx   There  is  
evidence  that  professional  typists  adopt  specific  reading  
strategies  to  optimize  performance  of  a  complex  motor  skill  
( Shaffer  , 1975) .sx   Typists  pay  more  attention  to  local  orthographic  
features  of  the  text  and  exhibit  an  'eye  -  hand'  span  that  is  
much  shorter  than  the  normal  eye-voice  span  ( Butsch  , 1932) .sx   Average  
fixation  duration  during  transcription  typing  is  much  longer  than  
normal  , and  average  saccade  extent  is  much  shorter  ( Inhoff  , Morris  , 
&  Calabrese  , 1986) .sx   We  hypothesize  that  these  habits  will  carry  
over  to  unconstrained  reading  -  in  which  case  , small  deviations  
from  intended  eye  position  will  be  particularly  disruptive  for  this  
group  of  subjects .sx   The  choice  of  an  appropriate  comparison  groups  
is  , however  , problematic .sx   Completely  asymptomatic  typists  were  
extremely  rare  in  the  population  sampled .sx   In  any  case  , it  is  not  
clear  that  such  a  group  would  comprise  and  effective  control  if  , as  
both  Shaffer  and  Inhoff  et  al.  suggest  , typists  , in  general  , 
exhibit  a  characteristic  reading  style .sx   In  the  present  study  , 
therefore  , comparisons  were  drawn  between  a  group  of  typists  and  a  
group  of  normal  readers  ( students) .sx   The  latter  group  permits  direct  
comparisons  to  be  made  between  performance  on  the  task  employed  in  
the  present  study  and  the  extensive  literature  on  eye  movement  
control  , which  has  almost  exclusively  made  use  of  this  population  
of  subjects .sx   Analysis  of  the  results  will  focus  on  differences  in  
reading  strategy  between  the  two  groups  and  on  the  effects  of  
flicker  for  each  group .sx   
  Method  
  Materials  and  Design .sx   A  set  of  60  items  was  
constructed  , 30  of  the  form  shown  in  ( 1  ) , where  the  stimulus  word  
had  occurred  in  the  sentence  , and  30  like  ( 2  ) , where  the  sentence  
contained  a  close  synonym  of  the  target .sx   
  1 .sx   The  novels  in  the  library  had  started  to  go  mouldy  with  the  
damp .sx   novels   .sx